New York Times Science
The New York Times
home
Classifieds
  Automobiles
Job Market
Real Estate
All Classifieds
News
  Quick News
NYT Front Page
Arts
Business
Health
International
National
New York Region
Obituaries
Politics
Science
Sports
Technology
Weather
Corrections
Special: Taxes
Opinion
  Editorials / Op-Ed
Readers' Opinions
Features
  Automobiles
Books
Cartoons
Crossword/Games
Job Market
Living
Magazine
Movies
Photos
Real Estate
Travel
Week in Review
Destinations
  Boston.com
College Times
Learning Network
New York Today
NYT Store
Shopping
Services
  Archives
Screensavers
Help Center
Media Kit
NYT Mobile
Our Advertisers
Newspaper
  Home Delivery
Customer Service
Your Profile
Review Profile
E-Mail Options
Log Out
Text Version
search Welcome, halpern25  
Sign Up for Newsletters  |  Log Out
  
Go to Advanced Search
E-Mail This Article Printer-Friendly Format
Most E-Mailed Articles

April 10, 2001

Why Mathematicians Now Care About Their Hat Color

By SARA ROBINSON

Multimedia

interactive_feature  Testing a Mathemetician's Mettle

Related Articles

Expanded Coverage

Science: Physical Science

Readers' Opinions

Join a Discussion on Science in the News

BERKELEY, Calif., April 9 — It takes a particularly clever puzzle to stump a mind accustomed to performing mental gymnastics.

So it's no ordinary puzzle that's spreading through networks of mathematicians like a juicy bit of gossip. Known as "the hat problem" in its most popular incarnation, this seemingly simple puzzle is consuming brain cycles at universities and research labs across the country and has become a vibrant topic of discussion on the Internet.

The reason this problem is so captivating, mathematicians say, is that it is not just a recreational puzzle to be solved and put away.

Rather, it has deep and unexpected connections to coding theory, an active area of mathematical research with broad applications in telecommunications and computer science.

In their attempts to devise a complete solution to the problem, researchers are proving new theorems in! coding theory that may have applications well beyond mathematical puzzles.

"This puzzle is unique since it connects to unsolved mathematical questions," said Dr. Joe Buhler, deputy director of the Mathematical Sciences Research Institute here and a hat problem enthusiast.

The hat problem goes like this:

Three players enter a room and a red or blue hat is placed on each person's head. The color of each hat is determined by a coin toss, with the outcome of one coin toss having no effect on the others. Each person can see the other players' hats but not his own.

No communication of any sort is allowed, except for an initial strategy session before the game begins. Once they have had a chance to look at the other hats, the players must simultaneously guess the color of their own hats or pass. The group shares a hypothetical $3 million prize if at least one player guesses correctly and no players guess incorrectly.

The same game can be pl! ayed with any number of players. The general problem is to fin! d a stra tegy for the group that maximizes its chances of winning the prize.

One obvious strategy for the players, for instance, would be for one player to always guess "red" while the other players pass. This would give the group a 50 percent chance of winning the prize. Can the group do better?

Most mathematicians initially think not. Since each person's hat color is independent of the other players' colors and no communication is allowed, it seems impossible for the players to learn anything just by looking at one another. All the players can do, it seems, is guess.

"I tell someone the problem and they think they don't have the conditions right," said Dr. Peter Winkler, director of fundamental mathematics research at Bell Labs of Lucent Technologies in Murray Hill, N.J. "But if you try to prove it's impossible, it doesn't quite work."

Mathematicians credit the problem to Dr. Todd Ebert, a computer science instructo! r at the University of California at Irvine, who introduced it in his Ph.D. thesis at the University of California at Santa Barbara in 1998.

Dr. Ebert said he discovered the problem's appeal only recently, when he offered extra credit to his students for solving a seven-player version he called the "seven prisoners puzzle."

Next thing he knew, the problem was posted on Internet news groups and in chat rooms. "I started getting e-mail from all over the country," Dr. Ebert said.

Meanwhile, Dr. Winkler, a well- known collector and distributor of clever puzzles, heard the problem from a colleague and spread it widely. It has cropped up at Microsoft Research in Redmond, Wash., at Hewlett-Packard Laboratories in Palo Alto, Calif., and at mathematics, statistics and computer science departments at universities across the country.

The problem has even spread to the Caribbean. ! At a workshop at a research institute in Barbados, one hardy g! roup of theoretical computer scientists stayed up late one rum- soaked night, playing a drinking game based on the puzzle.

It spread to Berkeley after Dr. Winkler bumped into Dr. Elwyn Berlekamp, a professor in the Berkeley math department, at a conference in New Orleans in January.

"I told him about the problem and next thing I knew he was leaving messages on my hotel phone saying, `Great problem, haven't gotten it yet,' then finally, `I got it,' " Dr. Winkler said. "I thought, with his knowledge of coding theory, he'd find that approach, and he didn't disappoint me."

Dr. Berlekamp, a coding theory expert, said he figured out the solution to the simplest case in about half an hour, but he saw the coding theory connection only while he was falling asleep that night.

"If you look at old things that you know from a different angle, sometimes you can't see them," he said.

The first thing Dr. Berlekamp saw was that in the three-player case, it is ! possible for the group to win three- fourths of the time.

Three-fourths of the time, two of the players will have hats of the same color and the third player's hat will be the opposite color. The group can win every time this happens by using the following strategy: Once the game starts, each player looks at the other two players' hats. If the two hats are different colors, he passes. If they are the same color, the player guesses his own hat is the opposite color.

This way, every time the hat colors are distributed two and one, one player will guess correctly and the others will pass, and the group will win the game. When all the hats are the same color, however, all three players will guess incorrectly and the group will lose.

"If you look at the total number of guesses made, it's still the case that half are right and half wrong," Dr. Winkler said. "You only make progress if, when players are guessing wrong, a great many are guessing wrong."

The strategy gets far more complicated for larger numbers o! f player s.

Still, it all comes down to making sure that most of the time no one is wrong and occasionally everyone is wrong at once.

As it turns out, this requirement can be perfectly met only when the number of players is one less than a power of two (three, seven, 15 and so on.)

For example, in the game with 15 players, there is a strategy for which the group is victorious 15 out of every 16 times they play.

This strategy can be described using elegant mathematical structures known as Hamming codes. Hamming codes, named after Richard Hamming, the mathematician who discovered them, are basic tools studied by engineering students all over the world.

Hamming codes straddle the boundary between two types of mathematical objects: error correcting codes and covering codes.

Error correcting codes, techniques for correcting errors in data sent across noisy channels, are used in everything from cell phones to compact discs. Covering codes can! be used to compress data so they take up less space in a computer's memory.

"Hamming codes are perfect structures, a lot like crystals, where you can't move an atom in them or they are completely destroyed," said Dr. Amin Shokrollahi, chief scientist at Digital Fountain, which uses coding theory to speed up Internet data transmissions. "When you take the hat problem apart and look at its core, you see what you need are exactly Hamming codes."

When the game is played with fewer than nine players, the optimal solution can be determined using various types of codes. For larger numbers that aren't one less than a power of two, a strategy designed around the Hamming code solution works closer and closer to 100 percent of the time as the number of players grows.

Dr. Hendrik Lenstra, a professor of mathematics at Berkeley, and Dr. Gadiel Seroussi, director of information theory research at HP Labs, have developed a new type of covering code to define an ev! en better strategy for large numbers of players.

While! their s trategy is the best so far, they don't know that it is always optimal. The optimal solution to the hat problem, for all numbers of players, is still unknown.

"We're still working on it," Dr. Seroussi said. "And as a consequence of working on this problem, we've got some results in coding theory that are interesting in and of themselves."

For now, researchers say, it seems unlikely that a solution will have immediate practical applications. Still, one never knows what the future might hold. "My experience is that any mathematics I've done is useful eventually," Dr. Seroussi said.

Practically useful or not, for some researchers the hat problem has interesting social implications. "I like problems that have philosophical punch lines," Dr. Berlekamp said, citing two life lessons that can be gleaned from the puzzle:

"The first is that it's O.K. to be wrong as long as you contrive not to be wrong alone," he said. "The other, more important lesson is! a need for teamwork that goes against the grain of most mathematicians. If the evidence suggests someone on your team knows more than you, you should keep your mouth shut.

"Most of us assume that each player's strategy is oriented toward him getting it right, and it's not. It's the whole team."

Home | Back to Science | Search | Help Back to Top

E-Mail This Article Printer-Friendly Format
Most E-Mailed Articles


NYTimes.com/health
Get the Latest Stories
Aging
Anatomy
Children's Health
Fitness
Genetics
Men's Health
Nutrition
Personal Health
Policy
Psychology
Women's Health
Health Navigator
Healthcare Provider Search
NYTimes.com/health


Copyright 2001 The New York Times Company | Privacy Information